Skip to main content

Exploring the typology of reasoning influencing university teachers’ language teaching and learning strategies

Abstract

Despite the critical role of reasoning in shaping language teaching and learning strategies in higher education, prior studies have either separately examined reasoning or focused on one specific type thereof and language teaching and learning strategies. To address this gap, this study took a comprehensive approach by examining various types of reasoning in language instruction and their alignment with language teaching strategies in higher education. It adopted a sequential mixed-methods design through two phases of data collection. The study instruments included a mixed-method questionnaire, interviews, classroom observations, and document analysis. The quantitative phase collected data from 357 university teachers of Arabic-language disciplines at the university level who completed a mixed-methods questionnaire using a 5-point Likert scale and brief written explanation. Exploratory factor analysis, descriptive and inferential analysis of variance and the Scheffé test were used to analyse the quantitative data. In the follow-up qualitative phase, 20 university teachers from the quantitative phase were observed in the classroom and interviewed, and their document tasks were analysed using thematic analysis. The results unveiled four distinct typologies of university teachers’ reasoning closely aligned with their language teaching and learning strategies: inductive, abductive, analogical, and deductive reasoning. Notably, these typologies were associated with cognitive, metacognitive, interactional, and Knowledge strategies. Furthermore, the findings highlighted the intricate interplay and mixture of these typologies, indicating that university teachers do not rely solely on one type or strategy. Finally, the conclusion section outlines critical areas for future research concerning reasoning and language teaching and learning strategies in higher education.

Introduction

Teachers can cultivate their reasoning about the value of a new pedagogical approach, and that reasoning may result in collusion or collision with the new approach (Byrne et al., 2019). This has implications for how the new approach or curriculum model is used in university teaching, either as a new approach to establish new teaching practices or as a tool that affects current practices. Teachers’ practices within a given approach are likely to reflect their reasoning on teaching and learning (Gifford, 1992), and their reasoning is fundamental in providing insight into effective, transformative practices of a new approach (Mulligan, 2015). Fung (2017) asserts that developing research-based educational approaches is not only feasible but also holds the potential for significant transformation. This transformation offers insights into new educational initiatives focused on research- and inquiry-based learning, with the ability to create environments that promote critical dialogue about educational values and openness to fresh ideas (Boden & Nedeva, 2010; Fung, 2017; Hoon & Looker, 2013).

In considering the pros and cons of various forms of reasoning and the inconclusive findings of the current research, some argue for choosing between deductive and inductive teaching methods (Ellis, 2016; Hayes et al., 2010; Housen et al., 2016; Sulaiman, 2012). In contrast, others propose rethinking language instruction, moving beyond the strict binary choice of purely deductive or inductive methods and advocating for abductive or analogical instruction (Behrens, 2017; Bybee, 2010; Holyoak & Morrison, 2012; Oh, 2008). No studies, however, have thoroughly explored and compared the differences among these reasoning types in terms of their influence on language teaching and learning processes, even in the broader literature focusing on teachers’ reasoning in language education, especially within the context of Arabic-speaking countries. Furthermore, uncertainty remains regarding whether these reasoning types can demonstrate varying effectiveness and language teaching strategies in such contexts. To address this gap, the current study sought to answer the following research questions:

  1. (1)

    Which types of reasoning do university teachers adopt in language teaching and learning?

  2. (2)

    What are university teachers’ language teaching and learning strategies?

  3. (3)

    How and to what extent do university teachers’ reasoning types relate to their language teaching and learning strategies?

Theoretical framework

This section first explores the theoretical foundations of reasoning in language teaching and learning and then provides a comprehensive literature review examining the connection between reasoning and language teaching and learning.

Reasoning in language teaching and learning

The diversity and inherent complexity of reasoning make it challenging to provide precise definitions or to delineate its constituent components. Nonetheless, using concrete insights drawn from substantial knowledge of reasoning as applied to a teaching approach empowers the researcher to grasp and address the intricacies of reasoning effectively. Reasoning encompasses the credibility of a claim, the supporting evidence, the method employed to gather that evidence and the attributes of the sources providing the evidence (Kuhn, 2010; Sandoval et al., 2014). Evans (2010) suggests that a greater depth of thought and analysis in a task or problem can lead to a broader spectrum of reasoning paths taken to reach the solution. One remarkable attribute of human language is its capacity to convey information effectively within a given context. Spoken and written expressions do not have to include every single detail, as listeners or readers can deduce the intended meanings by assuming that the words convey only pertinent information. These inferences in communication are contingent upon the shared presumption and reasoning that speakers provide information to the extent required by their shared knowledge and the specific task (Frank & Goodman, 2012).

Despite the fundamental role of reasoning, there is a significant divergence of opinion regarding whether prelinguistic infants possess the capacity for reasoning or whether it is an attribute exclusive to those with linguistic capabilities (Carey et al., 2020; Johnson & Ma, 1999). This debate forms part of a profound, longstanding discussion of whether abstract thinking precedes language or whether such thinking becomes possible only with the development of natural language in both evolutionary and individual growth (Hume, 2000). Despite this divergence of opinion, the progress made in psychology, philosophy and linguistics has equipped researchers with the means and methodologies to investigate empirical evidence concerning the connection between language and reasoning. This relationship is unique to those possessing linguistic capabilities (Grigoroglou & Ganea, 2022).

When teaching language, it is important to allow students to practice and explain the language tasks under investigation (Ellis et al., 2019). Research- and inquiry-based approaches effectively teach both linguistic content and the nature of language by engaging students in various scientific activities. The significance of engaging in research- and inquiry-based activities lies in their ability to promote learner involvement by encouraging learners to engage in scientific reasoning processes. Researchers emphasise that scientific explanations require three fundamental elements: a claim, supporting evidence and sound reasoning. Evidence comprises data that can strengthen a claim, and reasoning is employed to justify the relationships between the claim and the evidence (Fung, 2017).

Researchers in the field of language learning have also discussed various student practices that are based on different types of reasoning. Specifically, they have classified four types of reasoning in language learning: inductive, deductive, abductive and analogical. Induction is a form of reasoning that entails drawing general principles from specific instances, while deduction, conversely, involves deriving specific examples from general principles (Hayes et al., 2010). Deductive instruction can expedite the development of language learning by enabling learners to identify the structure and characteristics of language (Lardiere, 2004), and it facilitates the transformation of declarative knowledge into procedural and automated skills through practice; a grasp of the rules can indirectly enhance learning by making learners more adept at recognising language forms in subsequent input (Ellis, 2016). Guided induction instruction has been shown to foster a strong awareness of language structure and a noticeable activation of recently acquired knowledge (Housen et al., 2016). Nevertheless, various studies suggest that induction outperforms deductive instruction in various written tasks, such as sentence reconstruction and judgment of grammaticality with error correction (Housen & Kuiken, 2009; Sulaiman, 2012).

Abductive reasoning has emerged as an explicit type of reasoning as an alternative to the inductive and deductive traditions. In abductive reasoning, one draws conclusions based on the most reasonable explanation for one’s observations or evidenc (Jovanovic & Krneta, 2012). Burch (2006) clarifies that it not only leads to the best explanation but also involves clarifying or normalising previously unexpected information. Oh’s (2008) study outlines four phases of abductive reasoning: exploration, examination, selection and explanation. In language learning, abductive reasoning involves a dual process of observing particular language traits and formulating conjectures regarding their structure and purpose (Jovanovic & Krneta, 2012).

Recent developments in education promote empowering students to become skilled communicators who exhibit creativity and productivity in their language use. This capability is facilitated by using abstract cognitive processes, including categories, schemas, structures and rules (Bybee, 2010). The acquisition of these cognitive processes hinges on recognising analogies between different constructs, which constitutes analogical reasoning. Acquiring these cognitive processes involves two prerequisites for analogical reasoning that are equally necessary for incorporating new items into constructions: structural alignment and knowledge of relational similarity (Bybee, 2010; Leroy et al., 2012; Markman & Gentner, 1993). Analogical reasoning identifies similarities between topics, facilitating inferences and issue resolution; it often relies on shared resemblances and plays a crucial role in human cognition, encompassing language learning and problem-solving (Holyoak & Morrison, 2012). Analogical reasoning involves three core subprocesses: retrieval, mapping and evaluation. Retrieval refers to recalling similar situations from long-term memory when dealing with a current topic in working memory. Mapping aligns representations and draws inferences from two simultaneous cases in working memory. After mapping, the third process, evaluation, involves assessing the analogy and its inferences (Gentner, 2003). Behrens (2017) determined that analogical reasoning is a significant catalyst in language learning. It enables learners to assimilate new elements into established categories and expand these categories by drawing on similarities and relational analogies.

Language teaching and learning strategies

Research on language teaching and learning has identified several strategies, but defining and classifying language learning and teaching strategies requires further in-depth exploration to clarify their distinct characteristics and distinguish them from regular learning activities (Hinton, 2014). Cohen and Dörnyei (2002) outlines three categories of language teaching and learning strategies: language learning strategies encompassing learners’ thoughts and behaviours, language-use strategies and self-motivation strategies. In a study including 348 students at a language school in New Zealand, Griffiths (2003) observed that advanced students demonstrated a higher frequency of using strategies related to interacting with others to learn vocabulary, reading and language systems and that they more often exploited available resources than elementary students. Peacock and Ho (2003) conducted a study across various disciplines (including computer studies, engineering, English, math, primary education and science) and found that learners of English exhibited the highest frequency of strategies, including cognitive, metacognitive and social strategies.

Key findings on language teaching and learning strategies in linguistics studies reveal that those strategies also fall into the three categories of cognitive, metacognitive and social. Cognitive strategies involve mental processes for learning language (Van den Broek & Helder, 2017), metacognitive strategies involve planning, monitoring and evaluating one’s learning (Teng, 2020) and social strategies involve interacting and communicating with others to learn the language (Griffiths, 2018). Language teaching strategies should also reflect language’s role in expressing thoughts and producing meaning, which is the focus of recent research on language concepts (De Villiers, 2014).

Recent studies on language teaching and learning strategies have highlighted the constructivist approach to language learning (Suhendi, 2018), which posits that language learning is an active process in which the teacher should use practices that encourage students to construct continually and build their knowledge on what they have already learned. The task should be designed to facilitate extrapolation and fill in knowledge gaps (Aljohani, 2017; Quoc & Van, 2023).

Although research on identifying and characterising learning and teaching language strategies is extensive, some studies have adopted a survey approach, with collected data limited to self-reported practices that needed more verification and confirmation (Mills & Gay, 2016). The current study explores the reasoning behind language teaching and learning strategies. A mixed-methods design facilitates such investigations, as it enables the researcher to use quantitative methods to explore teachers’ reasoning and language teaching and learning strategies among a large number of participants and use qualitative methods to explore teachers’ reasoning and their language teaching and learning strategies in depth at the individual and group levels.

Methodology

This paper investigates how university teachers’ reasoning influences their Arabic-language teaching strategies, aiming to develop a conceptual and empirical understanding of various types of reasoning and strategies in Arabic-language teaching and learning. It also delves into the categorisation of reasoning in applying these strategies. Recent revisions to undergraduate and postgraduate programs in Saudi universities have included enhancements to Arabic-language programs to cultivate students’ research and analytical skills and teach current advancements in linguistic knowledge and contemporary linguistic thought through research- and inquiry-based learning. This study was conducted at a public university in Saudi Arabia with five campuses, all of which have Arabic-language colleges. All the university teachers and students who participated in the study were well-informed about the research objectives and ethical protocols. The study adopted a mixed-methods approach including quantitative and qualitative data collection (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

The sequential study design procedure

To investigate the existing reasoning that influences Arabic-language teaching and learning among university teachers, a mixed-methods questionnaire was distributed in the first stage to university Arabic-language teachers from the faculties of both Arabic language and education (N = 357). The questionnaire included close-ended questions on the participants’ background, demographic information and their reasoning regarding their current teaching and learning practices. The participants were asked to indicate the weakness or strength of each reasoning statement in language teaching and learning using a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (extremely weak) to 5 (extremely strong). Moreover, they were asked to provide a brief, open-ended written explanation or example of their rating and support for each statement. The procedure took approximately 20 min to complete (both the open- and close-ended items). The questionnaire’s close-ended statements were formulated on the basis of a variety of sources, encompassing studies on inductive reasoning in language (Hayes et al., 2010; Housen et al., 2016; Sulaiman, 2012), deductive reasoning in language (Ellis, 2016; Housen & Kuiken, 2009; Lardiere, 2004) and abductive and analogical reasoning in language (Behrens, 2017; Bybee, 2010).

The researcher refined the mixed-methods questionnaire items for contextual relevance, after which the questionnaire underwent a three-step field testing process. In the first step, the validity and reliability of the reasoning questionnaire were pilot-tested with 12 academics currently teaching in the Arabic-language department. In the second step, three psychologists and four education academics experienced in language teaching and learning reviewed the questionnaire item by item and made additional editorial revisions as necessary. Finally, the Cronbach’s alpha (α) reliability coefficient was used to measure the questionnaire’s reliability and to determine the internal consistency of the extracted factors (which included mixed inductive and abductive; mixed inductive, deductive and abductive; mixed analogical and abductive; and mixed inductive and deductive) as well as the overall item set (Table 5).

The study employed exploratory factor analysis (EFA) to condense a comprehensive set of data items into a more manageable number of factors that were subsequently used for further analysis. EFA is a technique to reveal the underlying relationships among scale items by identifying highly interconnected groups. It involves a series of sequential stages that may be categorised into four primary areas: evaluating the fundamental assumptions in EFA, assessing the data’s suitability for factor analysis, executing the factor extraction process and applying factor rotation and interpretation. Commencing this process, the researcher begin by evaluating the appropriateness of the study data for factor analysis. It is essential in EFA that the data meet specific prerequisites, including having interval variables, lacking outliers and having a substantial sample size, typically exceeding 150 participants (Pallant, 2020).

The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) provides two valuable statistical tools for assessing the suitability of scale items for EFA: the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) test, which evaluates the sample’s adequacy for factor analysis and has a recommended minimum value of 0.6 (Tabachnick et al., 2013), and Bartlett’s test, which examines the sphericity of the data and should yield a significant result (p < 0.05) to validate the appropriateness in factor analysis. These two tools were employed and yielded satisfactory results as presented in the quantitative results section.

Because the reasoning scales measured agreement levels using a 20-point verbal-frequency scale, the questionnaire can be categorised as having an interval variable. No outliers were identified among the items analysed in this study. Furthermore, the data set comprised 357 cases, a sample size sufficient for conducting factor analysis. Consequently, all the necessary conditions for conducting EFA were met in this study. A descriptive statistical analysis of extracted factors, including means and standard deviation, was then conducted for each factor. Inferential statistics from two tests—analysis of variance (ANOVA) and the Scheffé test—were also employed to assess the potential influence of syllabus groups on university teachers’ reasoning and teaching strategies.

In the follow-up qualitative phase, a purposefully selected group of 20 university teachers from the quantitative phase were sampled, interviewed and observed. Additionally, their document tasks were analysed to gain insights into their reasoning and language teaching and learning strategies in the classroom. The researcher thematically analysed the interviews, classroom observations and documents to deepen the results of exploring the typology of university teachers’ reasoning and their language teaching and learning strategies. The process of thematic analysis was automated using NVivo, a software designed for qualitative data analysis (Jackson et al., 2019). The constant comparative analysis method was followed to identify similarities and differences and to explore codes, themes, categories and associated dimensions (Glaser & Strauss, 2017).

During the inductive qualitative analysis process, following a five-step process outlined by Braun and Clarke (2006), the researcher analysed verbatim transcriptions of interviews, classroom observations, and documents. The researcher strove to maintain a reflective approach during the analysis, refraining from imposing personal interpretations on the data (Patton, 2014). The researcher read and listened to all the recorded interviews, classroom observations, and document transcriptions to gain a comprehensive understanding of the data before proceeding to any sorting or coding. In the second step, the researcher extracted descriptive phrases that pertained to participants’ explanations and practices of their reasoning and language teaching and learning strategies. In the third step, the researcher created preliminary codes for the data by segmenting and labeling the extracted phrases and identifying commonalities. In the fourth step, if necessary, the researcher formed categories from the codes by aggregating similar codes. In the fifth step, the themes were identified, where the codes and categories were compared and examined to identify the similarities and differences across the codes and categories.

Moreover, it was crucial to triangulate the researcher’s inferences, involving peer debriefing and ensuring inter-coder reliability (Miles et al., 2020). Therefore, an assistant professor, who was not involved in any of classroom observation, interviews, and document analysis, was recruited and trained to analysis their own. The trained rater and the researcher engaged in multiple discussions regarding the codes and themes derived from the data. The rater and the researcher juxtaposed the themes generated in the analysis with the original statements found in the transcriptions of classroom observations, interviews, and documents. This comparison aimed to provide clarification, elaboration, or challenges to the identified codes and themes throughout the data analysis process.

Results

In this section, the researcher presents the results from the quantitative phase, followed by the results from the qualitative phase.

Quantitative results

This study used a sequential mixed-methods design to investigate how university teachers’ reasoning influences their Arabic-language teaching strategies. The study aimed to explore the teachers’ types of reasoning, their Arabic-language teaching and learning strategies and the categorisation of reasoning in applying those strategies. This section presents the results and analysis of the quantitative data in two subsections: Preliminary Data Analysis and Quantitative Results, addressing the first question regarding university teachers’ types of reasoning (R1).

Preliminarily data analysis

After coding and entering the quantitative data into SPSS, preliminary data analysis was conducted, including checking for outliers and normality and conducting EFA.

Checking for outliers and normality

This study used SPSS to check for outliers and normality by generating histograms and box plots for the study’s main variables. These scores were converted to standardised scores to indicate the existence of significant outliers. The scores related to the dependent variables of the current study, including inductive, deductive, abductive and analogical reasoning, indicated the absence of any outliers and were within an acceptable range of − 4.0 and + 4.0. The distribution of the dependent variables can be seen in the histograms as shown in Fig. 2; the scores were normally distributed. The skewness and kurtosis values were also used to statistically examine the normality of distribution. The skewness and kurtosis values as shown in Table 1 were less than 1.0, indicating that the distributions of the dependent variables were normal.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Distribution of the Dependent Variables

Table 1 Descriptive Statistics for the Dependent Variables (N = 357)

Exploratory factor analysis

To assess the suitability of the data for factor analysis, principal component analysis (PCA) was conducted on 20 measured variables, which were the questionnaire items related to reasoning. The researcher examined the correlation matrix to determine whether the data were factorable. The correlation matrix indicated that a significant proportion of the correlation coefficients exceeded 0.3

In Table 2, it is evident that all additional criteria for the factor analysis were satisfied: the KMO measure of sampling adequacy surpassed 0.6 (KMO = 0.858), and Bartlett’s test of sphericity yielded statistical significance (χ2 [190] = 3012.931, p < 0.01), confirming the suitability of the data for factor analysis.

Table 2 KMO and Bartlett’s test for the reasoning scale

The findings also indicated that factor analysis could be conducted on the reasoning scale. The actual data and simulative data were subjected to parallel analysis using a syntax written in SPSS. As presented in Table 3, parallel analysis results provide further evidence to determine the number of factors.

Table 3 Eigen values of the actual data and the simulative data

Upon examination of Table 3, it is evident that the eigenvalue of the first factor in the actual dataset is 6.618194, whereas in the simulative dataset, it is 1.516579. The eigenvalue of the second factor in the actual dataset is 6. 2.522304, whereas it is 1.418801 in the simulative dataset. The eigenvalue of the third factor in the actual dataset is 1.350751, whereas it is 1.244579 in the simulative dataset. The eigenvalue of the fourth factor in the actual dataset is 1.291508, whereas it is 1.159579 in the simulative dataset. When transitioning from the fourth factor to the fifth, the situation changes. Consequently, the number of reasoning scale factors is decisively restricted to four because the eigenvalue of the simulative data for the fifth factor and subsequent factors is higher than that of actual data.

The number of factors determined with the support of parallel analysis is likely to be observed in a manner consistent with what is seen on the scree plot presented in Graphic 2.

Upon examination of the scree plot in Fig. 3, illustrating the actual data curve, it is obvious that the four-factor construct determined through eigenvalue examination is supported. The graphic shows that the first four factors have higher eigenvalues than the rest of the factors.

Fig. 3
figure 3

The Scree Plot of the Actual Data

In the analysis, 20 variables were included. Using Kaiser’s criterion, the PCA (as depicted in Table 4) extracted four factors through a converging rotation process that took seven iterations. These four factors collectively explain 57.723% of the variance.

Table 4 Total variance explained for the reasoning scale

Every item displayed loadings surpassing the acceptable threshold of > 0.5. Consequently, the reasoning scale is composed of four extracted factors; the details of these factors, including their corresponding variables and loadings, are shown in Table 5.

Table 5 Factors identified in PCA for the types of reasoning

As presented in Table 5, the EFA revealed four mixed types of reasoning among university teachers in Arabic-language teaching and learning: mixed inductive and abductive; mixed inductive, deductive and abductive; mixed analogical and abductive; and mixed inductive and deductive.

Factor 1 describes teachers’ reasoning practices as a blend of inductive and abductive reasoning. The statements confirm that the teachers used abductive reasoning practices to make unfamiliar information more apparent and understandable for their students. They also stressed the importance of seeking the best explanations when faced with incomplete understanding. Moreover, the teachers indicated inductive reasoning by acknowledging the significance of infinite word meanings when learning language. They also recognised their students’ biases in language learning, such as assuming words refer to whole objects rather than parts, and understood the impact of mutual exclusivity bias on language learning.

In Factor 2, teachers indicated a combination of inductive, deductive and abductive reasoning. They indicated abductive reasoning by the belief that learning should be based on providing data to the students and allowing them to convert it into a problem-solving process. They also indicated that the focus in language learning should facilitate two measurable phases: generating and testing hypotheses. Furthermore, they indicated inductive reasoning by agreeing that, in language learning, students should derive the general principles of language by encountering specific instances of phonemes, morphology, words and sentences. They also indicated that the input structure plays a significant role in language learning. Additionally, they indicated deductive reasoning by suggesting that language learning should begin with a broad concept or theory that is refined into hypotheses.

In Factor 3, the teachers indicated a combination of analogical and abductive reasoning. They strongly emphasised analogical reasoning by recognising the importance of students having and manipulating linguistic information, analysing structures and doing mapping based on relational similarities in language learning. Furthermore, they stressed that language learning should be grounded in structural alignment and knowledge of relational similarity. They also emphasised abductive reasoning by stating that language learning for students should be contextualised with real-life problems or examples.

In Factor 4, the teachers indicated a combination of inductive and deductive reasoning. They acknowledged that students should learn language through various processes, including observation, pattern detection and hypothesis formulation, as part of inductive reasoning. At the same time, they recognised that students learn language by first understanding rules and then applying them through examples and practice, which are parts of deductive reasoning practice. Overall, the teachers understood that language learning is a complex process that involves deriving specific examples from general principles.

Assessing the reliability of a research scale is a fundamental consideration before conducting any analysis. The evaluation of scale reliability involves scrutinising the internal consistency of the variables within a factor of the scale. This assessment pertains to the extent to which the instrument consistently measures what it is designed to measure as summarised by Cohen et al. (2002). Cronbach’s α was used to assess the internal consistency of the four extracted factors; the alpha values are listed in Table 6.

Table 6 Reliability coefficient of the four extracted factors of reasoning (N = 357)

The Cronbach’s α reliability coefficients span from 0.718 to 0.836 as shown in Table 5. DeVellis (1991) suggests that reliability levels between 0.70 and 0.80 are acceptable, whereas levels between 0.80 and 0.90 are considered highly acceptable. As per Cronbach (1951), a reliability coefficient (Cronbach’s α) exceeding 0.7 is typically deemed acceptable. Hence, the four extracted factors in this study exhibit satisfactory internal consistency.

As described in the previous section, EFA identified four extracted factors related to university teachers’ reasoning: mixed inductive and abductive; mixed inductive, deductive and abductive; mixed analogical and abductive; and mixed inductive and deductive. This subsection analyses these four factors that contribute to university teachers’ reasoning (Table 7).

Table 7 University teachers’ reasoning

The university teachers generally endorsed all reasoning types, indicating that teaching and learning language should include all types of reasoning (M = 3.89, SD = 0.40). The mean scores for all types ranged from 3.86 to 3.96. However, a close examination of the mean scores for the items of all types revealed that university teachers strongly agreed with the reasoning that teaching and learning language should seek the best clarification for incomplete understanding (M = 4.22, SD = 0.64), involve deriving specific examples from general principles (M = 4.05, SD = 0.75), make previously unknown information clearer and more readable (M = 4.01, SD = 0.67) and be based on knowledge of relational similarity (M = 4.01, SD = 0.66). In summary, the results show that, for each reasoning type, there was strong agreement among the participants.

Typology of reasoning by language syllabus

The participants were categorised into five syllabus groups: syntax, rhetoric, literature, linguistics and discourse. As indicated in Table 8, both ANOVA and the Scheffé test were employed to assess the potential influence of syllabus groups on the university teachers’ reasoning.

Table 8 Typology of reasoning by language syllabus group

A significant difference was found in the responses of the five syllabus groups concerning the mixed inductive and abductive reasoning typology (F [4,352] = 6.302, p < 0.05). Scheffé’s post hoc test indicated that the discourse syllabus group was statistically significant (p < 0.05), with the result showing that university teachers who followed a discourse syllabus rated the level of mixed inductive and abductive reasoning in language teaching and learning (M = 4.10, SD = 0.41) higher than university teachers who taught syntax, rhetoric, linguistics or literature.

Similarly, a significant difference was found in the responses of the five syllabus groups regarding the mixed analytical and abductive reasoning typology (F [4,352] = 25.774, p < .05). Scheffé’s post hoc test indicated that the discourse syllabus group was statistically significant (p < .05), revealing that university teachers who followed a discourse syllabus rated the level of mixed analytical and abductive reasoning in language teaching and learning (M = 4.52, SD = 0.33) higher than those who taught syntax, rhetoric, linguistics or literature.

In contrast, the ANOVA test results demonstrate that there were no significant differences between the five syllabus groups regarding the mixed inductive, deductive and abductive reasoning typology (F [4,352] = 0.840, p > 0.05) or the mixed inductive and deductive reasoning typology (F [4,352] = 2.152, p \(>\) 0.05). These findings suggest that the five syllabus groups perceived similar types of reasoning.

As explained in the Methodology section, five teachers from each category were interviewed, and curriculum documents were analysed to gain further insights into the reasoning behind their practices. These data are presented below in the Qualitative Results section.

Qualitative results

This section addresses the second and third research questions: university teachers’ language teaching and learning strategies (R 2), and the relationship between university teachers’ reasoning types and their language teaching and learning strategies (R 3). The four factors that were quantitatively identified as distinct reasoning practices are here more profoundly investigated by analysing university teachers’ reasoning, which informs their practices. This examination relies on data gathered from qualitative written explanations, interviews, observations and document analysis. Table 9 summarises the typology of university teachers’ reasoning, which will be examined individually (mixed inductive and abductive; mixed inductive, deductive and abductive; mixed analogical and abductive; and mixed inductive and deductive), and the corresponding language teaching and learning strategies.

Table 9 Typology of University Teachers’ Reasoning and Language Teaching and Learning Strategies

Mixed inductive and abductive reasoning

Saleh and Fhaed taught syntax, Saeed and Waleed focused on rhetoric and Assam specialised in teaching literature. They primarily identified with an inductive approach but also acknowledged their abductive nature. Saleh explained, ‘It makes sense to logically progress from specific concepts to broader ones, which can help students remember language topics and avoid rapidly forgetting’. Fhaed indicated that, based on reasoning, he tended to encourage students to think about concepts, words and meanings and that he could help them to recall and use those concepts, words and meanings later. Waleed remarked, ‘Inductive learning is a practical method for rhetoric subjects due to their prominent features, uniformity and ease of use, and [it promotes] cognitive thinking about concepts’. Similarly, Assam suggested that ‘embracing inductive reasoning facilitates a deeper understanding of the literature topics under study’. Naser indicated that students should reflect mentally and connect new concepts to previously acquired concepts and skills. Naser also indicated that he usually allowed his students to keep clarifying their ideas until they became familiar, even if the clarification was sometimes inaccurate. He justified this strategy by explaining that such practices helped students think critically about various ideas.

As indicated in Table 9, the central theme drawn from the qualitative analysis of written explanations, interviews and curriculum content analysis is that teachers with mixed inductive and abductive reasoning focused more on cognitive strategies in language teaching and learning. Saleh, Fhaed and Waleed used cognitive teaching strategies to help their students remember language topics. Table 10 presents an overview of relevant extracts from the transcripts that show the reasoning that informed the teaching strategies of Saleh, Fhaed and Waleed. The table presents curriculum content and teaching episodes from Saleh, who was selected randomly as an example to represent the other two teachers.

Table 10 Selection of curriculum content/teaching episodes of saleh

As shown in Table 10, the cognitive strategy used in the lesson was to check on the recall of grammar rules. For instance, in line 1, Salah asked students a cognitive explanatory question—‘How do we explain this sentence structure? “The lesson is learnt by students”’—to elicit the first responses from students. He then moved from this simple question to employ cognitive thinking questions. He asked his students to explain and provide reasons—‘How and why do we use passive voice?’ (line 3),—asked for in-depth explanations—‘This is not enough. We need more in-depth explanation’ (line 5)—and asked them to clarify and differentiate between grammar rules ‘So, how does active voice differ from passive voice?’ (line 9).

These findings indicate that Salah initiated his lesson by asking questions regarding the topic without considering the student’s prior knowledge. As described by Van den Broek and Helder (2017), such initiated questions have become routine in teaching and stimulate cognitive processes.

Mixed inductive, deductive and abductive reasoning

Ahamed, Anas and Khalid taught linguistics, Ali, Yazid and Basim taught syntax and rhetoric and Masfar specialised in teaching literature. They identified with a combination of inductive, deductive and abductive reasoning. Ahamed and Khalid emphasised that they normally planned for student learning and believed that this is the priority approach. Anas indicated that teachers need to look at how students learn rather than focusing on their thinking, which helps them know when their students better understand the text. Yazid and Masfar highlighted that current students should be able to evaluate their learning processes. Yazid explained that comment by emphasising the importance of encouraging students first to assess their thinking levels. They should then improve their thinking while engaging in various literature, research and other texts. Similarly, Masfar highlighted the importance of keeping students updated with knowledge, but he recommended that students go through the process of self-evaluating their current thinking proficiency. Basim indicated that he preferred to motivate his students to use high-quality resources. However, he strongly indicated that such an approach required assisting them in managing their own learning and access to resources.

Table 9 shows that the qualitative analysis reveals that the participants who combined mixed inductive, deductive and abductive reasoning focused on metacognitive strategies. Ahamed and Khalid employed their teaching strategies by planning for learning, Masfar and Yazid employed their practices by managing thinking, Anas employed his practices by focusing on learning styles and Basim employed the approach of managing students’ independent learning. Table 11 presents an overview of relevant extracts that illustrate this group. The selection of curriculum content and teaching episodes is from Yazid, who was selected randomly as an example of this group.

Table 11 Selection of curriculum content/teaching episodes of Yazid

As demonstrated in Table 11, Yazid’s approach incorporated metacognitive strategies. For instance, in the lesson ‘emphasis phrases’, he began by asking students to develop their lesson plan with clear objectives, such as ‘learn how to use language to confirm ideas’ and ‘learn sorts of words or phrases to convince others’. He used positive language, such as ‘excellent’, to motivate students to achieve their learning objectives and select appropriate learning strategies. He also initiated the lesson by referring to the topic ‘emphasis phrases’ but without any more details, which is an example of common deductive practice.

After encouraging students to set their learning objective for the lesson, Yazid engaged them in inductive reasoning by asking them to draw general principles regarding emphasis phrases from specific sentences as in line 7. This activity was supported abductively by identifying students’ areas of weakness and conjectures—‘Don’t worry, just let me know what [you] guess first, even if you make a mistake’ (line 9)—and asking them to reflect on their thinking (line 11), which led the teacher to adjust his teaching strategy (line 12).

In line 16, Yazid also asked students to look at the previous sentences without emphasis phrases, omitting each emphasis phrase from the sentences. This strategy is considered useful for developing concepts.

Mixed analogical and abductive reasoning

Mohannad and Salem taught linguistics, and Hasan and Samir taught literature and discourse. They identified with a combination of analogical and abductive reasoning. Mohannad, Mohannad and Salem emphasised that they allowed their students to engage in communicative practice. Mohannad claimed that employing a communicative approach aligns with the overall purpose of language learning, which prepares students to engage in effective and meaningful communication while learning language topics.

When we look at the overarching goal of learning any language, it is undoubtedly to assist our students in communicating effectively and meaningfully with each other. Therefore, we must align our teaching with this objective by encouraging students to learn linguistic rules while communicating and interacting with others. (Mohannad, Int. L 34)

Salem also focused on interaction and a communicative approach when teaching functional grammar. Salem’s beliefs reflected his awareness that this communicative approach pays more attention to the functional and structural aspects of language. He stated:

I always tend to facilitate interaction and communication between students, because such a strategy is compatible with functional grammar and other structural aspects of language. (Salem, Int. L 28)

In contrast, Hasan’s teaching strategies aligned a communicative approach with contextualising learning because, as they justified, both the communicative approach and contextualising learning support language use. Both endeavoured to guide students’ classroom practices to communicate with each other but not focus on one language structure. For example, Hasan stated:

I encourage my students to learn by communicating with each other and advise them to bring real-life examples and use any language rules or information they can think about. I avoid task-based learning, because such [a] practice forces students to use certain tasks, grammar or purpose, which is against our natural way of learning and using language. (Hasan, Int. L 16)

Similarly, Samir promoted a communicative approach and contextualising learning, as he believed these practices enable students to apply what they have learned in real-life situations and focus more on the formation and functionality of language. He stated:

We should keep our practice in the way of using a communicative approach and learning in the real context. I can see how such practices help my students use what they have learned in real circumstances and shift their attention to how language is formed and works in different contexts. (Samir, Int. L 16)

Table 12 presents an overview of relevant extracts that illustrate this group. The selection of curriculum content and teaching episodes is from Hasan, who was randomly selected as an example of this group.

Table 12 Selection of curriculum content/teaching episodes of hasan

As demonstrated in Table 12, Hasan’s approach incorporated interactional strategies. For instance, he began by emphasising a communicative approach, urging students to prioritise comprehension while communicating as in lines 1 and 3. He justified this by stating that ‘a student learning a language may make grammatical mistakes; however, their statements would still be comprehensible to the listener’ (line 3). This does not imply neglecting the importance of learning grammar. Hasan advised his students to condense key points from their grammatical references, memorise and thoroughly comprehend them and then apply those grammar rules in various oral and written contexts (line 3).

Hasan also promoted student interaction within contextualising learning. To underscore the importance of understanding the context and culture, he tasked students with studying public spaces to comprehend the meanings of culture, values, social habits and interaction patterns and to incorporate such knowledge into their learning process (line 7).

Mixed inductive and deductive reasoning

Talal and Faiz taught linguistics and discourse, and Bender and Jasser taught literature, discourse and rhetoric. They identified with a combination of inductive and deductive reasoning. All the participants in this group integrated the exploration of existing knowledge into their teaching strategy, a practice that helps teachers recognise their students’ thinking levels and enables them to build upon them throughout the learning progression. For example, Talal claimed that ‘beginning with an exploration of existing knowledge contributes to the growth of knowledge and ideas’ (Talal, Int. L 23). Faiz expanded on this strategy by emphasising the importance of bridging the gap between current and new knowledge. He stated:

We can’t teach students how to learn, but we can motivate them within their existing understanding to help them comprehend the relationship between their current understanding of language concepts and new concepts. (Faiz, Int. L 23)

Bender and Jasser believed that employing inductive and deductive processes helped them create knowledge by establishing a connection between prior knowledge and new learning. For example, Bender stated:

Knowing students’ existing knowledge helps to establish new learning, and that can also motivate students to create knowledge in a way that addresses a sense of dissatisfaction in the students’ prior or current knowledge. (Bender, Int. L 16)

Jasser also indicated that using a strategy to elicit students’ current knowledge and alternative conceptions could promote conceptual development, create new ideas and enable them to reconsider their existing knowledge in the learning process.

As indicated in Table 9, the central theme drawn from the qualitative analysis of written explanations, interviews and curriculum content analysis is that teachers with mixed inductive and deductive reasoning focused more on knowledge strategies in language teaching and learning. Table 13 presents an overview of relevant extracts that illustrate this group. The selection of curriculum content and teaching episodes is from Jasser, who was randomly selected to represent the other two teachers.

Table 13 Selection of curriculum content/teaching episodes of Jasser

As shown in Table 13, Jasser’s approach involved using knowledge strategies. For instance, he started by using open-ended questions to explore the students’ existing knowledge as observed in line 1. Subsequently, he bridged the gap between current and new knowledge by expanding on and building on students’ previous responses as illustrated in line 8. This process included circling back to the first student’s response about the discussion, elaborating further and drawing comparisons with the argument.

Jasser also used a sequence of tasks to encourage students to create knowledge through research, define and apply concepts in written and supportive essays and engage in classroom discussions for generating and reflecting on new knowledge (line 10).

Discussion

This study contributes to the literature by (a) identifying types of reasoning in university language teaching and learning, (b) exploring university language teaching and learning strategies and (c) synthesising two research strands to investigate the relationship between types of reasoning and language teaching and learning strategies by employing a sequential mixed-methods design that built on the quantitative and qualitative results. The present study identified four types of reasoning among university teachers to characterise the four groups discussed in this section. Table 14 summarises the components that form each group and addresses the correspondence between university teachers’ reasoning and their language teaching and learning strategies. The quantitative results indicate that the university teachers strongly endorsed all reasoning types, and the reasoning types that constitute the four groups are interrelated and mixed, which suggests that the reasoning of university teachers is not always one type or another; rather, it seems that all types of reasoning are important and useful when used together in language teaching and learning. These findings align consistently with those of transformative scholars (Behrens, 2017; Bybee, 2010; Holyoak & Morrison, 2012; Oh, 2008), who reject the binary either/or choice in reasoning within language instruction.

Table 14 Integrated results matrix

The relationship between university teachers’ reasoning and their language teaching and learning strategies emerged from synthesising the quantitative and qualitative findings regarding their reasoning and their teaching strategies. Group 1 combined inductive and abductive reasoning, indicating that the teachers used abductive reasoning practices by making unfamiliar information more apparent and understandable for their students and encouraging students to seek clarification when faced with incomplete understanding. This validates previous research emphasising the importance of abductive reasoning as an explicit type of reasoning that not only leads to the best explanation but also involves the clarification of previous information (Burch, 2006).

Additionally, the teachers employed inductive reasoning practices by acknowledging the importance of infinite word meanings when learning a language task. They also recognised their student’s biases in language learning, such as assuming words refer to whole objects rather than parts, and understood the impact of mutual exclusivity bias on the language learning process. This is in line with previous studies (Giorgou Tzampazi, 2019) that show the significance of mutual exclusivity bias as the principle that guides inductive reasoning in the presence of known words and grammar instruction. The qualitative results indicate that the university teachers in this group used cognitive strategies, including recalling language concepts and grammar rules, connecting new concepts to prior concepts, promoting deeper understanding and fostering critical thinking. The types of reasoning and cognitive strategies that Group 1 demonstrated tend to correspond to what is highlighted in Klauer and Phye’s review study (2008), which concludes that inductive reasoning promotes the use of cognitive processing.

This study’s quantitative findings also reveal significant differences between the five syllabus groups (syntax, rhetoric, linguistics, literature and discourse) concerning the mixed inductive and abductive reasoning typology. The results show that university teachers who taught a discourse syllabus rated the level of mixed inductive and abductive reasoning higher than university teachers who taught syntax, rhetoric, linguistics or literature. Thus, university teachers who taught discourse tended to employ mixed inductive and abductive reasoning. These findings are consistent with those of previous studies (Kuhn & Modrek, 2018; Kuhn et al., 2015) that indicate that choosing multiple types of reasoning is preferred when teaching argumentative discourse.

Group 2 primarily employed a form of reasoning that combines inductive, deductive and abductive principles. They employed abductive reasoning by providing students with data and allowing them to convert it into problems within focused language learning that facilitated two measurable phases: generating and testing hypotheses. They also employed inductive reasoning practices, indicating that students can derive general principles of language by encountering specific instances of phonemes, morphology, words and sentences and emphasising the importance of the structure of the input in language learning and acquisition. This result aligns with the findings of Housen et al. (2016), who stress the significance of guided induction instruction, wherein teachers structure student language learning. Deductive reasoning practices suggest the importance of beginning language learning with a broad concept or theory that is refined into hypotheses. This finding is corroborated by Hayes et al. (2010), who highlight that the characteristics of deduction involve deriving specific examples from general principles. The qualitative results indicate that the university teachers in Group 2 applied metacognitive strategies, including planning for learning, selecting appropriate learning strategies, managing thinking and developing concepts, which is consistent with previous studies (Teng, 2020).

Group 3 employed a form of reasoning that involves a combination of analogical and abductive reasoning. The analogical reasoning highlights the importance of students having and manipulating linguistic information, analysing structures and doing mapping based on structural alignment and knowledge of relational similarity in language learning. This finding aligns with the perspectives of those who emphasise the analogical reasoning process (Holyoak & Morrison, 2012). The qualitative results indicate that the university teachers in Group 3 applied interactional strategies, including learning by interaction, promoting a communicative approach and contextualising learning. This result corresponds to what Griffiths (2018) describes as ‘social teaching strategies’.

This study’s quantitative findings also reveal significant differences between the five syllabus groups (syntax, rhetoric, linguistics, literature and discourse) concerning the mixed analogical and abductive reasoning typology. The result show that university teachers who taught a discourse syllabus rated the level of mixed analogical and abductive reasoning in language teaching and learning higher than university teachers who taught syntax, rhetoric, linguistics or literature. These findings are consistent with those of previous studies (Kuhn & Modrek, 2018; Kuhn et al., 2015) indicating that, in teaching argumentative discourse, there is no dominant type of reasoning. Therefore, choosing multiple types of reasoning is preferred when teaching discourse.

Group 4 employed a combination of inductive and deductive reasoning in their teaching practices. They acknowledged that students learn language through various processes, such as observation, pattern detection and hypothesis formulation. This finding agrees with previous research (Ellis, 2016) that advocates for inductive reasoning practices. Along with inductive reasoning practices, the teachers highlighted the importance of students learning language by first understanding rules and then applying them through examples and practice and engaging in a complex process that involves deriving specific examples from general principles. These conceptions are part of deductive reasoning practices and align with the findings of previous research (Hayes et al., 2010). The qualitative results indicate that the teachers in Group 4 applied knowledge strategies, including exploring existing knowledge, bridging the gap between current and new knowledge and creating knowledge. This result aligns with the findings of recent studies (Aljohani, 2017; Quoc & Van, 2023) suggesting that the focus in language learning should move to designed tasks that facilitate extrapolation and fill in knowledge gaps.

Conclusion

This study employed a sequential mixed-methods design through two phases of data collection. Phase 1 included a mixed-methods questionnaire with 357 Arabic-language teachers. Phase 2 involved interviews, classroom observations and document analysis with 20 Arabic-language teachers to explore their reasoning and teaching strategies.

The present study offers several important findings. First, it shows that teachers strongly endorsed all reasoning types as important, which reflects their willingness to apply all types of reasoning in language teaching and learning. Second, the reasoning types that constitute the four groups identified in this study are interrelated and mixed, which indicates that university teachers’ reasoning is not dominated by one type but includes multiple types of reasoning in language teaching and learning. A third significant finding is that the results indicate a correspondence between university teachers’ reasoning and their language teaching and learning strategies. These findings indicate links between mixed inductive and abductive reasoning and cognitive strategies. Furthermore, connections were found between mixed inductive, deductive and abductive reasoning and metacognitive strategies. Additionally, the findings indicate links between mixed analogical and abductive reasoning and interactional strategies as well as between mixed inductive and deductive reasoning and knowledge strategies. Further research could apply the methodology used in this study to explore teachers’ reasoning and teaching strategies in other disciplines.

Fourth, among the quantitative findings of this study was the significant difference discovered in the responses of the five syllabus groups regarding the mixed analogical and abductive reasoning typology and the mixed inductive, deductive and abductive reasoning typology. The results show that university teachers who taught a discourse syllabus rated the level of those types of reasoning higher than those who taught syntax, rhetoric, linguistics or literature. Further research on reasoning and teaching discourse could be conducted to provide an in-depth understanding of these typologies and the relationship between reasoning and teaching discourse.

Availability of data and materials

The data supporting this study’s findings are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

References

  • Aljohani, M. (2017). Principles of “constructivism” in foreign language teaching. Journal of Literature and Art Studies, 7(1), 97–107.

    MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Behrens, H. (2017). The role of analogy in language processing and acquisition. In The changing English language: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp. 215–239).

  • Boden, R., & Nedeva, M. (2010). Employing discourse: Universities and graduate ‘employability.’ Journal of Education Policy, 25(1), 37–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burch, R. (2006). Charles Sanders Peirce. In Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy.

  • Bybee, J. (2010). Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. M., Evans, J. S. B., & Newstead, S. E. (2019). Human reasoning: The psychology of deduction. Psychology Press.

  • Carey, S., Leahy, B., Redshaw, J., & Suddendorf, T. (2020). Could it be so? The cognitive science of possibility. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(1), 3–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A. D., & Dörnyei, Z. (2002). Focus on the language learner: Motivation, styles, and strategies. An Introduction to Applied Linguistics, 14, 170–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L., Manion, L., & Morrison, K. (2002). Research methods in education. Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. psychometrika, 16(3), 297–334.

  • De Villiers, J. (2014). What kind of concepts need language? Language Sciences, 46, 100–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeVellis, M. (1991). Reliability test of attitudinal instruments. Paper presented at the annual research conference, university of Minnesota, Minneapolis.

  • Ellis, R., Skehan, P., Li, S., Shintani, N., & Lambert, C. (2019). Task-based language teaching: Theory and practice. Cambridge University Press.

  • Ellis, N. C. (2016). Salience, cognition, language complexity, and complex adaptive systems. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(2), 341–351.

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. (2010). Intuition and reasoning: A dual-process perspective. Psychological Inquiry, 21(4), 313–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, M. C., & Goodman, N. D. (2012). Predicting pragmatic reasoning in language games. Science, 336(6084), 998–998.

    Article  ADS  MathSciNet  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fung, D. (2017). A connected curriculum for higher education. UCL Press.

  • Gentner, D. (2003). Analogical reasoning, psychology of. Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science, 1, 106–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gifford, N. K. T. (1992). The relationship of moral reasoning level of instructors to their teaching style and adult student perception of the learning environment. University of Nebraska–Lincoln.

  • Giorgou Tzampazi, S. (2019). The effects of deductive, inductive and a combination of both types of grammar instruction in pre-sessional classes in higher education.

  • Glaser, B., & Strauss, A. (2017). Discovery of grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative research. Routledge.

  • Griffiths, C. (2018). The strategy factor in successful language learning: The tornado effect (Vol. 121). Multilingual Matters.

  • Griffiths, C. (2003). Patterns of language learning strategy use. System, 31(3), 367–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grigoroglou, M., & Ganea, P. A. (2022). Language as a mechanism for reasoning about possibilities. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 377(1866), 20210334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, B. K., Heit, E., & Swendsen, H. (2010). Inductive reasoning. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(2), 278–292.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hinton, L. (2014). Language revitalization and language pedagogy: New teaching and learning strategies. In Applied linguists needed (pp. 41–52). Routledge.

  • Holyoak, K. J., & Morrison, R. G. (2012). The Oxford handbook of thinking and reasoning. Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hoon, C. H., & Looker, P. (2013). On the margins of SoTL discourse: An Asian perspective. Teaching and Learning Inquiry, 1(1), 131–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Housen, A., Simoens, H., Cerezo, L., Caras, A., & Leow, R. P. (2016). The effectiveness of guided induction versus deductive instruction on the development of complex Spanish gustar structures. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(2).

  • Housen, A., & Kuiken, F. (2009). Complexity, accuracy, and fluency in second language acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 30(4), 461–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (2000). A treatise of human nature. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, K., Bazeley, P., & Bazeley, P. (2019). Qualitative data analysis with NVivo. Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. E., & Ma, P. (1999). Understanding language teaching: Reasoning in action. Heinle & Heinle.

  • Jovanovic, A. S., & Krneta, G. G. Z. (2012). Abductive reasoning and second language learning. Journal of Language Teaching and Research, 3(2), 306–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klauer, K. J., & Phye, G. D. (2008). Inductive reasoning: A training approach. Review of Educational Research, 78(1), 85–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. (2010). What is scientific thinking and how does it develop? The Wiley‐Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 497–523).

  • Kuhn, D., & Modrek, A. (2018). Do reasoning limitations undermine discourse? Thinking & Reasoning, 24(1), 97–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., Ramsey, S., & Arvidsson, T. S. (2015). Developing multivariable thinkers. Cognitive Development, 35, 92–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lardiere, D. (2004). Carroll, SE (2001). Input and evidence: The raw material of second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26(3), 461–468.

  • Leroy, S., Parisse, C., & Maillart, C. (2012). Analogical reasoning in children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 26(4), 380–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, A. B., & Gentner, D. (1993). Structural alignment during similarity comparisons. Cognitive Psychology, 25(4), 431–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miles, H., Huberman, A. M., & Saldana, J. (2020). Qualitative data analysis: A methods sourcebook (4th ed.). Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mills, G. E., & Gay, L. R. (2016). Educational research: Competencies for analysis and applications. Pearson.

  • Mulligan, J. (2015). Looking within and beyond the geometry curriculum: Connecting spatial reasoning to mathematics learning. ZDM Mathematics Education, 47, 511–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oh, P. S. (2008). Adopting the abductive inquiry model (AIM) into undergraduate earth science laboratories. In Science education in the 21st century (pp. 263–277).

  • Pallant, J. (2020). SPSS survival manual: A step by step guide to data analysis using IBM SPSS. McGraw-Hill Education (UK).

  • Patton, M. Q. (2014). Qualitative research & evaluation methods: Integrating theory and practice. Sage publications.

  • Peacock, M., & Ho, B. (2003). Student language learning strategies across eight disciplines. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 13(2), 179–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quoc, N. L., & Van, L. H. (2023). Enhancement of EFL learners’ lexical retention: The role of social constructivism. Cogent Education, 10(1), 2223811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sandoval, W. A., Sodian, B., Koerber, S., & Wong, J. (2014). Developing children’s early competencies to engage with science. Educational Psychologist, 49(2), 139–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suhendi, A. (2018). Constructivist learning theory: The contribution to foreign language learning and teaching. KnE Social Sciences, 87–95.

  • Sulaiman, S. (2012). The effectiveness of inductive and deductive approach in teaching adverbs of frequency among low achievers. Universiti Teknologi Malaysia.

  • Tabachnick, B. G., Fidell, L. S., & Ullman, J. B. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (Vol. 6). Pearson.

  • Teng, F. (2020). The role of metacognitive knowledge and regulation in mediating university EFL learners’ writing performance. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching, 14(5), 436–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Broek, P., & Helder, A. (2017). Cognitive processes in discourse comprehension: Passive processes, reader-initiated processes, and evolving mental representations. Discourse Processes, 54(5–6), 360–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

The author gratefully acknowledges the assistance of the Arabic language departments at Umm Al-Qura University, Saudi Arabia, in facilitating the data collection. The author received no financial support for this article’s research, authorship, or publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Abdulmajeed Alghamdi designed the study, contributed to the literature review, collected and analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Abdulmajeed Alghamdi.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval and consent to participate

The author declares that no potential conflict of interest is reported in this article’s research.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Alghamdi, A. Exploring the typology of reasoning influencing university teachers’ language teaching and learning strategies. Asian. J. Second. Foreign. Lang. Educ. 9, 51 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40862-024-00273-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40862-024-00273-1

Keywords